Catching the scent: Dal pilot study shows dogs can smell trauma

- April 4, 2024

Ivy, a red gold retriever seen left, and Callie, a German Shepherd, found their work for Dal study 'inherently motivating,' says researcher. (Provided photos)
Ivy, a red gold retriever seen left, and Callie, a German Shepherd, found their work for Dal study 'inherently motivating,' says researcher. (Provided photos)

Dogs’ sensitive noses can detect the early warning signs of many potentially dangerous medical conditions, like an impending seizure or sudden hypoglycemia in diabetics. Now, scientists have found evidence that assistance dogs might even be able to sniff out an oncoming post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) flashback.

"PTSD service dogs are already trained to assist people during episodes of distress," said Laura Kiiroja in Dal's Department of Psychology and Neuroscience and first author of a paper in Frontiers in Allergy.

"However, dogs are currently trained to respond to behavioral and physical cues. Our study showed that at least some dogs can also detect these episodes via breath."

PTSD arises from exposure to a catastrophic event: symptoms include re-experiencing that event, hyperarousal, avoiding reminders and cognitive or mood issues. Among other forms of assistance, dogs can help patients by alerting them to and interrupting episodes when their companions are struggling with symptoms. If dogs could respond to stress markers on the breath, they could potentially interrupt episodes at an earlier stage, making their interventions more effective.

Understanding scent profiles
 

All humans have a 'scent profile' of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) — molecules emitted by the body in secretions like sweat — influenced by our genetics, age, activities and other variables. There is some evidence that dogs may be capable of detecting VOCs linked to human stress. However, no studies have investigated whether dogs could learn to detect VOCs associated with PTSD symptoms.

This was a multidisciplinary collaboration between Dr. Sherry Stewart’s clinical psychology lab and Dr. Simon Gadbois’s canine olfaction lab at Dalhousie. "Neither lab could have done this work on their own. We brought together two distinct sets of expertise," says Kiiroja.

The scientists recruited 26 people as scent donors. Participants were also taking part in a study about how people react to reminders of trauma they had experienced; 54 per cent met the diagnostic requirements for PTSD. To donate scents, they attended sessions where they were reminded of their traumatic experiences while wearing different facemasks. One facemask provided a calm breath sample that acted as the control and another, which was worn while the participants recalled their trauma, provided a target breath sample. Participants also completed a questionnaire about their stress levels and their emotions.

Meet the dogs


The scientists also recruited 25 pet dogs to train in scent detection. Only two were skilled and motivated enough to complete the study: Ivy and Callie.

"Both Ivy and Callie found this work inherently motivating," said Kiiroja. "Their limitless appetite for delicious treats was also an asset! In fact, it was much harder to convince them to take a break than to commence work! Callie in particular made sure there was no dilly-dallying."

Ivy and Callie were trained to recognize the target odour from pieces of the facemasks, achieving 90 per cent accuracy in discriminating between a stressed and a non-stressed sample. They were then presented with a series of samples, one sample at a time, to see if they could still accurately detect the stress VOCs. In this second experiment, Ivy achieved 74 per cent accuracy and Callie achieved 81 per cent accuracy.


Ivy with Laura Kiiroja.

Comparing Callie and Ivy's successful identifications with the human participants' self-reported emotions revealed that Ivy's performance correlated with anxiety, whereas Callie's correlated with shame.

"Although both dogs performed at very high accuracy, they seemed to have a slightly different idea of what they considered a 'stressed' breath sample," said Kiiroja. "We speculated that Ivy was attuned to sympathetic-adreno-medullar axis hormones (like adrenaline) and Callie was oriented to the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis hormones (like cortisol). This is important knowledge for training service dogs, as alerting to early-onset PTSD symptoms requires sensitivity to sympathetic-adreno-medullar axis hormones."

Validating study findings


Next, the team plans to carry out experiments to confirm the involvement of the sympathetic-adreno-medullar axis.

"With 40 sample sets, ours is a proof-of-concept study that needs to be validated by studies with larger sample sizes," cautioned Kiiroja. "In addition to enrolling more participants, validation studies should collect samples from a higher number of stressful events to confirm dogs' ability to reliably detect stress VOCs in the breath of one human across different contexts.


Callie.


Comments

All comments require a name and email address. You may also choose to log-in using your preferred social network or register with Disqus, the software we use for our commenting system. Join the conversation, but keep it clean, stay on the topic and be brief. Read comments policy.

comments powered by Disqus