Detecting deception

Dal study says motivated lie catchers make mistakes

- October 2, 2007

Dr. Michael Woodworth
"What's really intriguing about the study is that the people who were very motivated to catch liars tended also to be more confident," says Michael Woodworth. (Photo courtesy of Michael Woodworth)

It took 48 years for accused murderer Steven Truscott to have the court set aside the conviction against him, and even then, it refused to clear his name by declaring his innocence.  

Last month, as the Ontario Court of Appeal unanimously overturned the original conviction and declared the case a miscarriage of justice, the judges went on to say that “the court is not satisfied that the appellant has been able to demonstrate his factual innocence.”

Guilty or innocent? That’s the foundation of every trial and underscores the importance of detecting deception.

According to a new Dalhousie research study, discovering dishonesty doesn’t depend so much on picking up on stereotypical clues, but on how badly you want to. The study came to the surprising conclusion that the more motivated you are, the more likely you are to make mistakes.  

A paper based on the results has just been published in Legal and Criminological Psychology, a journal of the British Psychological Society.

“What’s really intriguing about this study is that the people who were very motivated to catch liars tended also to be more confident,” says Michael Woodworth, who worked on the study as a graduate student in Stephen Porter’s forensic psychology lab at Dalhousie University. He now teaches forensic psychology and conducts research as an assistant professor at the University of British Columbia’s Okanagan campus. “And when they were overconfident, they didn’t take as much time to reflect on their decisions and tended to make a lot more mistakes.”

 

'Hanged by the neck until you are dead'

"Inspector Graham, who arrested Truscott, was highly confident in Truscott's guilt and saw that his 'eyes were filled with anger, not fear' as the judge announced that he would be hanged for the crime. On the other hand, journalists described the same reaction in a remarkably different light: 'his eyes filled with tears, Steven Truscott gasped in the dock' and 'the boy simply turned pale.'

— The Globe and Mail on Steven Truscott's original trial

Researchers observed that overzealous lie catchers seemed to have a kind of “tunnel vision” that has been seen in so many police investigations and miscarriages of justice in Canadian courts. In the Truscott case, for example, police were convinced within 24 hours of finding a body that they had their killer and didn’t investigate other possible suspects.

To use a courtroom example, a judge may instantaneously assess a defendant as being untrustworthy based on certain behaviors, such as fidgeting or crying on the stand. Although the defendant may indeed be innocent — the fidgeting is a sign of nervousness rather than guilt — the damage is already done.

Which isn’t to say that lie catchers can’t get better. Researchers also came to the conclusion that feedback — accurate or not — could improve the lie catcher’s performance.

“That’s the kicker,” says Dr. Woodworth. “Accurate or inaccurate, it didn’t seem to make a lot of difference, and this is one area that needs to be teased apart in future studies. But it appears that feedback of any sort made people reflect on their decisions and that’s what made the real improvement.”

The study was done using 150 undergraduate students. The student observers were shown videotapes in which 12 people described highly stressful personal experiences, half of which were completely falsified.

Some participants were offered monetary incentives and told that only a small number of people were particularly skilled at catching liars.

Study results suggest that it’s important for lie-catchers to monitor their motivation level to ensure their enthusiasm isn't clouding their judgments. Dr. Woodworth also says it may be useful for professionals engaged in deception detection — judges, lawyers, police officers, customs officers, border guards — to regularly discuss their judgments with colleagues and solicit feedback as a way of reevaluating their decision-making strategies.

“The fact is that a lot of people in the legal system are extremely motivated to find guilt, especially when the pressures are huge and there’s public outcry,” says Dr. Woodworth. “But being motivated isn’t everything. It might not be beneficial at all.”


Comments

All comments require a name and email address. You may also choose to log-in using your preferred social network or register with Disqus, the software we use for our commenting system. Join the conversation, but keep it clean, stay on the topic and be brief. Read comments policy.

comments powered by Disqus